Advertisement
Review Article| Volume 26, ISSUE 4, P728-737, June 2023

Download started.

Ok

Efficacy of Transcranial Alternating Current Stimulation in the Enhancement of Working Memory Performance in Healthy Adults: A Systematic Meta-Analysis

  • Nicole R. Nissim
    Correspondence
    Address correspondence to: Nicole R. Nissim, PhD, Goddard Laboratory, 3710 Hamilton Walk, Philadelphia, PA 19104.
    Affiliations
    Laboratory for Cognition and Neural Stimulation, Department of Neurology, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA

    Moss Rehabilitation Research Institute, Einstein Medical Center, Elkins Park, PA, USA
    Search for articles by this author
  • Darrian C. McAfee
    Affiliations
    Laboratory for Cognition and Neural Stimulation, Department of Neurology, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
    Search for articles by this author
  • Shanna Edwards
    Affiliations
    Laboratory for Cognition and Neural Stimulation, Department of Neurology, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
    Search for articles by this author
  • Amara Prato
    Affiliations
    Laboratory for Cognition and Neural Stimulation, Department of Neurology, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
    Search for articles by this author
  • Jennifer X. Lin
    Affiliations
    Laboratory for Cognition and Neural Stimulation, Department of Neurology, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
    Search for articles by this author
  • Zhiye Lu
    Affiliations
    Laboratory for Cognition and Neural Stimulation, Department of Neurology, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
    Search for articles by this author
  • H. Branch Coslett
    Affiliations
    Laboratory for Cognition and Neural Stimulation, Department of Neurology, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA

    Moss Rehabilitation Research Institute, Einstein Medical Center, Elkins Park, PA, USA
    Search for articles by this author
  • Roy H. Hamilton
    Affiliations
    Laboratory for Cognition and Neural Stimulation, Department of Neurology, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA

    Moss Rehabilitation Research Institute, Einstein Medical Center, Elkins Park, PA, USA
    Search for articles by this author
Open AccessPublished:February 08, 2023DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurom.2022.12.014

      Abstract

      Background

      Transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS)—a noninvasive brain stimulation technique that modulates cortical oscillations in the brain—has shown the capacity to enhance working memory (WM) abilities in healthy individuals. The efficacy of tACS in the improvement of WM performance in healthy individuals is not yet fully understood.

      Objective/Hypothesis

      This meta-analysis aimed to systematically evaluate the efficacy of tACS in the enhancement of WM in healthy individuals and to assess moderators of response to stimulation. We hypothesized that active tACS would significantly enhance WM compared with sham. We further hypothesized that it would do so in a task-dependent manner and that differing stimulation parameters would affect response to tACS.

      Materials and Methods

      Ten tACS studies met the inclusion criteria and provided 32 effects in the overall analysis. Random-effect models assessed mean change scores on WM tasks from baseline to poststimulation. The included studies involved varied in stimulation parameters, between-subject and within-subject study designs, and online vs offline tACS.

      Results

      We observed a significant, heterogeneous, and moderate effect size for active tACS in the enhancement of WM performance over sham (Cohen’s d = 0.5). Cognitive load, task domain, session number, and stimulation region showed a significant relationship between active tACS and enhanced WM behavior over sham.

      Conclusions

      Our findings indicate that active tACS enhances WM performance in healthy individuals compared with sham. Future randomized controlled trials are needed to further explore key parameters, including personalized stimulation vs standardized electroencephalography frequencies and maintenance of tACS effects, and whether tACS-induced effects translate to populations with WM impairments.

      Keywords

      Introduction

      Working memory (WM)—the capacity for temporary storage and manipulation or reorganization of information held online—facilitates many higher-level cognitive functions (eg, learning, language, problem solving). As a largely frontal lobe-mediated cognitive process, WM is linked to fluid intelligence and is involved in decision-making and goal-directed behaviors that are fundamental to everyday life.
      • Jaeggi S.M.
      • Buschkuehl M.
      • Jonides J.
      • Perrig W.J.
      Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory.
      ,
      • Au J.
      • Sheehan E.
      • Tsai N.
      • Duncan G.J.
      • Buschkuehl M.
      • Jaeggi S.M.
      Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory: a meta-analysis.
      Unfortunately, this critical cognitive ability typically decreases with age, even among healthy older adults, which can negatively affect quality of life, limit functional independence, and increase mortality rates among the older adult population.
      • Schupf N.
      • Tang M.X.
      • Albert S.M.
      • et al.
      Decline in cognitive and functional skills increases mortality risk in nondemented elderly.
      ,
      • Mograbi D.C.
      • Faria Cde A.
      • Fichman H.C.
      • Paradela E.M.P.
      • Lourenço R.A.
      Relationship between activities of daily living and cognitive ability in a sample of older adults with heterogeneous educational level.
      Recent years have witnessed growing interest in the use of noninvasive brain stimulation (NIBS) techniques as interventions aimed at cognitive enhancement, maintenance of cognition, or slowing of age-related cognitive changes.
      • Nissim N.R.
      • O’Shea A.
      • Indahlastari A.
      • et al.
      Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation paired with cognitive training on functional connectivity of the working memory network in older adults.
      • Nissim N.R.
      • O’Shea A.
      • Indahlastari A.
      • et al.
      Effects of in-scanner bilateral frontal tDCS on functional connectivity of the working memory network in older adults.
      • Ditye T.
      • Jacobson L.
      • Walsh V.
      • Lavidor M.
      Modulating behavioral inhibition by tDCS combined with cognitive training.
      • Park S.H.
      • Seo J.H.
      • Kim Y.H.
      • Ko M.H.
      Long-term effects of transcranial direct current stimulation combined with computer-assisted cognitive training in healthy older adults.
      • Meinzer M.
      • Lindenberg R.
      • Antonenko D.
      • Flaisch T.
      • Flöel A.
      Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation temporarily reverses age-associated cognitive decline and functional brain activity changes.
      • Dhaynaut M.
      • Cappon D.
      • Paciorek R.
      • et al.
      Effects of modulating gamma oscillations via 40Hz transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) on Tau PET imaging in mild to moderate Alzheimer’s Disease.
      • Benussi A.
      • Cantoni V.
      • Cotelli M.S.
      • et al.
      Exposure to gamma tACS in Alzheimer’s disease: a randomized, double-blind, sham-controlled, crossover, pilot study.
      Most WM NIBS studies to date have used transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS), which applies a direct current that affects the resting membrane potential of neurons.
      • Senkowski D.
      • Sobirey R.
      • Haslacher D.
      • Soekadar S.R.
      Boosting working memory: uncovering the differential effects of tDCS and tACS.
      In contrast, transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) is a form of NIBS that safely and painlessly alters cortical oscillations, rhythmic patterns of electrophysiological activity of the brain, in a frequency-specific manner.
      • Antonenko D.
      • Faxel M.
      • Grittner U.
      • Lavidor M.
      • Floël A.
      Effects of transcranial alternating current stimulation on cognitive functions in healthy young and older adults.
      ,
      • Elyamany O.
      • Leicht G.
      • Herrmann C.S.
      • Mulert C.
      Transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS): from basic mechanisms towards first applications in psychiatry.
      Similarly to tDCS, tACS influences cortical excitability and brain activity but differs from tDCS by the application of alternating sinusoidal electrical currents (in-phase or antiphase) using electrodes placed over the scalp.
      • Antal A.
      • Paulus W.
      Transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS).
      ,
      • Alekseichuk I.
      • Turi Z.
      • Veit S.
      • Paulus W.
      Model-driven neuromodulation of the right posterior region promotes encoding of long-term memories.
      The mechanism of action involves entrainment of endogenous neural oscillations as a function of rhythmic shifts in membrane potentials to the set frequency of stimulation,
      • Herrmann C.S.
      Modeling-informed tACS allows shaping oscillatory activity in specific brain networks.
      • Vosskuhl J.
      • Strüber D.
      • Herrmann C.S.
      Transcranial alternating current stimulation. entrainment and function control of neuronal networks.
      • Vosskuhl J.
      • Strüber D.
      • Herrmann C.S.
      Non-invasive brain stimulation: A paradigm shift in understanding brain oscillations.
      • Fröhlich F.
      • McCormick D.A.
      Endogenous electric fields may guide neocortical network activity.
      • Klink K.
      • Paßmann S.
      • Kasten F.H.
      • Peter J.
      The modulation of cognitive performance with transcranial alternating current stimulation: a systematic review of frequency-specific effects.
      differing from tDCS. Frequency of tACS can be applied at a standard value across participants, or personalized (electroencephalography [EEG]-informed) to an individual’s peak frequency of interest. tACS can synchronize (or desynchronize, depending on phase) neural oscillations to modulate cortical rhythms that underlie cognitive processes, manipulate brain activity, and affect behavior.
      • Ruhnau P.
      • Neuling T.
      • Fuscá M.
      • Herrmann C.S.
      • Demarchi G.
      • Weisz N.
      Eyes wide shut: transcranial alternating current stimulation drives alpha rhythm in a state dependent manner.
      • Neuling T.
      • Ruhnau P.
      • Weisz N.
      • Herrmann C.S.
      • Demarchi G.
      Faith and oscillations recovered: on analyzing EEG/MEG signals during tACS.
      • Feurra M.
      • Pasqualetti P.
      • Bianco G.
      • Santarnecchi E.
      • Rossi A.
      • Rossi S.
      State-dependent effects of transcranial oscillatory currents on the motor system: what you think matters.
      In addition to being dependent on phase and frequency of stimulation, effects of tACS on brain oscillations have also been shown to be task/state-dependent, in that the activities one engages in at the time of stimulation directly influence the effects elicited by stimulation.
      • Ruhnau P.
      • Neuling T.
      • Fuscá M.
      • Herrmann C.S.
      • Demarchi G.
      • Weisz N.
      Eyes wide shut: transcranial alternating current stimulation drives alpha rhythm in a state dependent manner.
      ,
      • Feurra M.
      • Pasqualetti P.
      • Bianco G.
      • Santarnecchi E.
      • Rossi A.
      • Rossi S.
      State-dependent effects of transcranial oscillatory currents on the motor system: what you think matters.
      • Nakazono H.
      • Ogata K.
      • Kuroda T.
      • Tobimatsu S.
      Phase and frequency-dependent effects of transcranial alternating current stimulation on motor cortical excitability.
      • Pahor A.
      • Jaušovec N.
      The effects of theta and gamma tacs on working memory and electrophysiology.
      Furthermore, evidence suggests that multiple sessions of tACS entrainment can elicit enduring neuroplastic changes and subsequent physiologic and behavioral aftereffects,
      • Antal A.
      • Paulus W.
      Transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS).
      ,
      • Herrmann C.S.
      Modeling-informed tACS allows shaping oscillatory activity in specific brain networks.
      ,
      • Antal A.
      • Paulus W.
      • Nitsche M.A.
      Principle and mechanisms of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS).
      ,
      • Tavakoli A.V.
      • Yun K.
      Transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) mechanisms and protocols.
      underscoring the therapeutic potential of tACS.
      • Elyamany O.
      • Leicht G.
      • Herrmann C.S.
      • Mulert C.
      Transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS): from basic mechanisms towards first applications in psychiatry.
      Neural oscillations play an important role in a variety of cognitive functions, including WM.
      • Pahor A.
      • Jaušovec N.
      The effects of theta and gamma tacs on working memory and electrophysiology.
      Mounting evidence from EEG and magnetoencephalography studies indicates that WM is associated with synchronous activity across multiple frequency bands independently (eg, theta, alpha, beta, and gamma) in addition to cross-frequency coupling between theta and gamma (eg, theta-nested gamma).
      • Roux F.
      • Uhlhaas P.J.
      Working memory and neural oscillations: α-γ versus θ-γ codes for distinct WM information?.
      • Hoy K.E.
      • Bailey N.
      • Arnold S.
      • et al.
      The effect of γ-tACS on working memory performance in healthy controls.
      • Reinhart R.M.G.
      • Nguyen J.A.
      Working memory revived in older adults by synchronizing rhythmic brain circuits.
      • Lisman J.E.
      • Jensen O.
      The θ-γ neural code.
      • Sauseng P.
      • Klimesch W.
      • Heise K.F.
      • et al.
      Brain oscillatory substrates of visual short-term memory capacity.
      Theta oscillations are thought to be involved in the organization of sequentially ordered WM items, whereas gamma-band oscillations appear to correspond to general maintenance of WM information.
      • Axmacher N.
      • Henseler M.M.
      • Jensen O.
      • Weinreich I.
      • Elger C.E.
      • Fell J.
      Cross-frequency coupling supports multi-item working memory in the human hippocampus.
      ,
      • Fell J.
      • Axmacher N.
      The role of phase synchronization in memory processes.
      Previous research has suggested that cross-frequency coupling between low (theta) and high (gamma) frequencies enables the processing of information held in WM, including the sequential ordering and maintenance of stimuli.
      • Tavakoli A.V.
      • Yun K.
      Transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) mechanisms and protocols.
      ,
      • Roux F.
      • Uhlhaas P.J.
      Working memory and neural oscillations: α-γ versus θ-γ codes for distinct WM information?.
      ,
      • Sauseng P.
      • Griesmayr B.
      • Freunberger R.
      • Klimesch W.
      Control mechanisms in working memory: a possible function of EEG theta oscillations.
      ,
      • Alekseichuk I.
      • Turi Z.
      • Amador de Lara G.
      • Antal A.
      • Paulus W.
      Spatial working memory in humans depends on theta and high gamma synchronization in the prefrontal cortex.
      Disruption of theta frequency has been shown to impair WM performance.
      • Alekseichuk I.
      • Pabel S.C.
      • Antal A.
      • Paulus W.
      Intrahemispheric theta rhythm desynchronization impairs working memory.
      A growing body of research suggests that tACS can enhance cognitive processes that underlie WM function in healthy individuals.
      • Klink K.
      • Paßmann S.
      • Kasten F.H.
      • Peter J.
      The modulation of cognitive performance with transcranial alternating current stimulation: a systematic review of frequency-specific effects.
      ,
      • Hoy K.E.
      • Bailey N.
      • Arnold S.
      • et al.
      The effect of γ-tACS on working memory performance in healthy controls.
      ,
      • Reinhart R.M.G.
      • Nguyen J.A.
      Working memory revived in older adults by synchronizing rhythmic brain circuits.
      ,
      • Jaušovec N.
      • Jaušovec K.
      Increasing working memory capacity with theta transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS).
      ,
      • Bullard B.
      • Levina V.
      • Grover S.
      • Reinhart R.
      Effects of transcranial alternating current stimulation on visual cognition: a systematic review and meta-analysis.
      For instance, a study in healthy participants found that tACS applied at individualized theta frequency during a WM task increased short-term memory capacity in the active vs sham group.
      • Vosskuhl J.
      • Huster R.J.
      • Herrmann C.S.
      Increase in short-term memory capacity induced by down-regulating individual theta frequency via transcranial alternating current stimulation.
      Recent research has suggested that active frontotemporal theta tACS can enhance WM performance in healthy older adults to levels comparable with those of young adults.
      • Reinhart R.M.G.
      • Nguyen J.A.
      Working memory revived in older adults by synchronizing rhythmic brain circuits.
      There is also evidence that specific frequency bands are relevant to different features of WM, such as the positive association between gamma-band frequency (> 40 Hz) and performance at higher cognitive loads of WM tasks in healthy individuals.
      • Roux F.
      • Uhlhaas P.J.
      Working memory and neural oscillations: α-γ versus θ-γ codes for distinct WM information?.
      ,
      • Hoy K.E.
      • Bailey N.
      • Arnold S.
      • et al.
      The effect of γ-tACS on working memory performance in healthy controls.
      ,
      • Honkanen R.
      • Rouhinen S.
      • Wang S.H.
      • Palva J.M.
      • Palva S.
      Gamma oscillations underlie the maintenance of feature-specific information and the contents of visual working memory.
      ,
      • Howard M.W.
      • Rizzuto D.S.
      • Caplan J.B.
      • et al.
      Gamma oscillations correlate with working memory load in humans.
      The ability to manipulate cortical oscillations to a standardized frequency or personalized to an individual makes tACS a promising tool to alter the brain activity that underlies cognition.
      However, although studies using tACS to enhance WM have shown promise, there exists variability in the response to tACS that is not yet well understood.
      • Guerra A.
      • Asci F.
      • Zampogna A.
      • et al.
      Gamma-transcranial alternating current stimulation and theta-burst stimulation: inter-subject variability and the role of BDNF.
      Moreover, the efficacy of tACS in enhancing WM in healthy individuals has not yet been explored in sufficiently large cohorts to be considered definitive. Therefore, the aim of this meta-analysis was to assess the efficacy of tACS in the enhancement of WM performance (eg, accuracy or reaction time) in healthy participants. Potential moderators of treatment effects such as stimulation parameters (including frequency of stimulation, number of sessions, duration, stimulation region, subject-specific frequency vs standard frequency), WM task demands (eg, cognitive load), verbal vs spatial tasks, and participant demographics were also explored, to determine potential moderator effects on tACS response. We hypothesized that active tACS vs sham would significantly enhance WM task performance and improve behavioral outcomes. We also hypothesized that tACS-induced effects on behavioral performance would be revealed in a task-dependent manner.

      Materials And Methods

      This systematic meta-analysis was conducted and reported in accordance with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) guidelines.
      • Moher D.
      • Liberati A.
      • Tetzlaff J.
      • Altman D.G.
      PRISMA Group
      Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses: the PRISMA statement.
      ,
      • Page M.J.
      • McKenzie J.E.
      • Bossuyt P.M.
      • et al.
      The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews.

      Literature Search Strategy

      One reviewer (Nicole R. Nissim) carried out literature searches to identify studies assessing tACS in the context of WM performance in healthy individuals. Articles were identified through a computerized literature search using the data bases Embase, PubMed, Web of Science, Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials, and clinicaltrials.gov. The search terms included for titles, abstracts, and keywords were “transcranial alternating current stimulation” OR “tACS” OR “oscillatory activity” AND “working memory” OR “executive function” OR “cognition.” The search was limited to published research articles between January 1960 and March 2022 written in English. Using this approach, we identified 509 articles from Embase, PubMed, Web of Science, and Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials, and 24 records from ClinicalTrials.gov. The PRISMA flow diagram displays the procedures for study identification as seen in Figure 1. Additional thorough manual reviews of the articles were performed as described in Figure 1.
      Figure thumbnail gr1
      Figure 1Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses flow chart for the search and selection of studies. N/A, not applicable.

      Eligibility: Inclusion/Exclusion

      Articles were eligible for inclusion if the studies they reported 1) enrolled healthy human subjects; 2) involved administration of tACS either online or offline (eg, during or before behavioral assessments); 3) assessed WM performance before, during, or after stimulation; and 4) had > two participants. Between-subject studies with active vs sham trials and baseline data were included, in addition to within-subject crossover study designs. The rationale for requiring pre- and poststimulation data was to increase the validity and stability of WM performance across studies that assessed different cognitive aspects of WM and included heterogeneous stimulation protocols. The following factors excluded articles from meta-analysis: 1) case studies of a single participant; 2) studies involving clinical populations; 3) review articles; 4) studies involving nonalternating waveforms of transcranial electrical stimulation (tES); 5) studies that involved pharmacologic or other additional interventions; or 6) studies that assessed tACS only in motor or sensory contexts. Common reasons for excluding articles were duplication within the literature search, tACS applications in noncognitive domains, studies involving other brain stimulation techniques, review articles, or limited statistical reporting (eg, conference abstracts).

      Literature Data Extraction

      Manuscripts (titles, abstracts, and full texts) were independently screened by four of the authors (Nicole R. Nissim, Darrian C. McAfee, Shanna Edwards, and Amara Prato). Any disagreements during the selection process were resolved through collaborative discussion and consensus. The final selected studies are summarized in Table 1 and the study demographics in Table 2. For articles that met the inclusion criteria, the information extracted was author and publication year, study design, sample size, participant demographics (eg, age, sex, and education when reported), cognitive task, cognitive domain of the task (ie, verbal vs spatial WM task), mean performance and SDs (accuracy and/or reaction time) at baseline, during tACS, or after stimulation to calculate change score, and stimulation parameters including duration, frequency band, number of sessions, electrode location and size, region of stimulation (ie, frontal vs parietal vs frontoparietal vs frontotemporal), hemisphere (ie, bilateral vs left vs right), and personalized vs standard frequency.
      Table 1Data Summary of Included Studies in the Meta-Analysis.
      Author, yearStudy designSession numberDuration (min)Stimulation frequencyElectrode location (anode, cathode[s])Electrode sizeConcurrent taskTaskTask domainMeasureOutcome
      Meiron and Lavidor,
      • Meiron O.
      • Lavidor M.
      Prefrontal oscillatory stimulation modulates access to cognitive control references in retrospective metacognitive commentary.
      2014
      Between-subject1204.5 Hz; thetaBilateral DLPFC (F3/F4)4 × 4 cmOnlineN-BackVerbalAccuracy, RTWM accuracy significantly improved
      Jaušovec and Jaušovec,
      • Jaušovec N.
      • Jaušovec K.
      Increasing working memory capacity with theta transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS).
      2014
      Within-subject215personalized; thetaLeft parietal (P3), right eyebrow5 × 7 cm; 10 × 7 cmOfflineVisual array comparison taskSpatialAccuracy, RTWM storage capacity significantly improved
      Jaušovec et al,
      • Jaušovec N.
      • Jaušovec K.
      • Pahor A.
      The influence of theta transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) on working memory storage and processing functions.
      2014
      Within-subject215personalized; thetaLeft parietal (P3), right eyebrow5 × 7 cmOfflineCorsi block tapping task (FW/BW); Digit span (FW/BW)Verbal-spatialAccuracyWM storage capacity significantly improved
      Hoy et al,
      • Hoy K.E.
      • Bailey N.
      • Arnold S.
      • et al.
      The effect of γ-tACS on working memory performance in healthy controls.
      2015
      Within-subject12040 Hz; gammaLeft frontal (F3), right supraorbital area5 × 7 cmOfflineN-BackVerbalAccuracyLarger performance improvement in active vs sham, not statistically significant
      Borghini et al,
      • Borghini G.
      • Candini M.
      • Filannino C.
      • et al.
      Alpha oscillations are causally linked to inhibitory abilities in ageing.
      2018
      Within-subject42010 Hz; alphaBilateral parietal (P3/P4)5 × 7 cmOnlineRetro-cue WM paradigmSpatialAccuracyWM recall accuracy significantly improved
      Jones et al,
      • Jones K.T.
      • Arciniega H.
      • Berryhill M.E.
      Replacing tDCS with theta tACS provides selective, but not general WM benefits.
      2019
      Within-subject1154.5 Hz; thetaRight DLPFC (F4), right parietal (P4)5 × 5 cmOfflineN-BackObjectAccuracyObject WM significantly improved
      Bender et al,
      • Bender M.
      • Romei V.
      • Sauseng P.
      Slow theta tACS of the right parietal cortex enhances contralateral visual working memory capacity.
      2019
      Within-subject2124 Hz; thetaRight parietal (P4); Oz, Cz, and T819.6 cm2; 4.9 cm2 returnOnlineDelayed match-to-sampleVisuo-spatialAccuracyWM storage capacity significantly improved
      Reinhart and Nguyen,
      • Reinhart R.M.G.
      • Nguyen J.A.
      Working memory revived in older adults by synchronizing rhythmic brain circuits.
      2019
      Within-subject125personalized; thetaLeft frontal (F3), left temporal (T3)12 mm diameter, Ag/AgClOnlineChange detection taskObjectAccuracy, RTWM accuracy significantly improved
      Biel et al,
      • Biel A.L.
      • Sterner E.
      • Röll L.
      • Sauseng P.
      Modulating verbal working memory with fronto-parietal transcranial electric stimulation at theta frequency: does it work?.
      2022
      Between-subject1146 Hz; thetaLeft frontal (F3), left parietal (P3); Cz, focal2.5 cm diameterOnlineDelayed Letter Recognition TaskVerbalAccuracy, RTPerformance in demanding task significantly improved
      Thompson et al,
      • Thompson L.
      • Khuc J.
      • Saccani M.S.
      • Zokaei N.
      • Cappelletti M.
      Gamma oscillations modulate working memory recall precision.
      2021
      Within-subject12035 Hz; gammaBilateral parietal (P3/P4)5 × 7 cmOnlineRetro-cue WM paradigmVisuo-spatialAccuracyWM recall accuracy significantly improved
      Table 2Study Sample Demographics.
      Author, ySample sizeAge (y)Percent femaleEducation (mean year)
      Meiron and Lavidor,
      • Meiron O.
      • Lavidor M.
      Prefrontal oscillatory stimulation modulates access to cognitive control references in retrospective metacognitive commentary.
      2014
      2421.510012.67 Active; 12.43 Sham
      Jaušovec and Jaušovec,
      • Jaušovec N.
      • Jaušovec K.
      Increasing working memory capacity with theta transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS).
      2014
      1220.666.6
      Jaušovec et al,
      • Jaušovec N.
      • Jaušovec K.
      • Pahor A.
      The influence of theta transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) on working memory storage and processing functions.
      2014
      1220.575
      Hoy et al,
      • Hoy K.E.
      • Bailey N.
      • Arnold S.
      • et al.
      The effect of γ-tACS on working memory performance in healthy controls.
      2015
      1829.35016.23
      Borghini et al,
      • Borghini G.
      • Candini M.
      • Filannino C.
      • et al.
      Alpha oscillations are causally linked to inhibitory abilities in ageing.
      2018
      2569.14416.2
      Jones et al,
      • Jones K.T.
      • Arciniega H.
      • Berryhill M.E.
      Replacing tDCS with theta tACS provides selective, but not general WM benefits.
      2019
      3824.566
      Bender et al,
      • Bender M.
      • Romei V.
      • Sauseng P.
      Slow theta tACS of the right parietal cortex enhances contralateral visual working memory capacity.
      2019
      1421.985
      Reinhart and Nguyen,
      • Reinhart R.M.G.
      • Nguyen J.A.
      Working memory revived in older adults by synchronizing rhythmic brain circuits.
      2019
      4268.85217
      Biel et al,
      • Biel A.L.
      • Sterner E.
      • Röll L.
      • Sauseng P.
      Modulating verbal working memory with fronto-parietal transcranial electric stimulation at theta frequency: does it work?.
      2022
      2421.358.3
      Thompson et al,
      • Thompson L.
      • Khuc J.
      • Saccani M.S.
      • Zokaei N.
      • Cappelletti M.
      Gamma oscillations modulate working memory recall precision.
      2021
      5124.158.8

      Data Analyses

      The Comprehensive Meta-Analysis software version 3 (CMA, Englewood, NJ) was used to perform analyses. To account for heterogeneity across studies due to differences in methods and sample characteristics, the random-effects model approach was used for all analyses.
      • Riley R.D.
      • Higgins J.P.T.
      • Deeks J.J.
      Interpretation of random effects meta-analyses.
      The main outcome measures, accuracy, or reaction time on WM assessments were defined as the mean percent correct response or mean latency (millisecond) determined from the change score (baseline to poststimulation). For all studies, change in performance was calculated by comparing the mean accuracy or latency achieved before with during or after active and/or sham stimulation. If the means and SDs were not reported, effect sizes were calculated from reported univariate F-tests, t-statistics, or p values. In the event studies reported that active vs sham differences were not statistically significant, but did not report the direction of the effect, the direction was coded as negative to provide more conservative effect size estimates. Effect sizes were classified as small (d ≥ 0.20), medium (d ≥ 0.5) or large (d ≥ 0.80), corresponding to previous conventions.
      • Cohen J.
      Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences.
      To determine whether statistical significance was achieved, CIs and z-transformations of the effect size were used. The criterion for statistical significance was achieved for mean effects within the 95% CI that did not span zero, providing evidence that tACS has a reproducible, robust effect on WM in healthy adults. Cochran Q-statistic, which computes the sum of the squared deviations from the estimate of each study from the overall meta-analysis estimate,
      • Hedges L.V.
      • Olkin I.
      Statistical Methods for Meta-Analysis.
      was used to assess how much of the total variability could be attributed to heterogeneity among the selected studies or whether variations in findings were due to chance alone.
      • Grant J.
      • Hunter A.
      Measuring inconsistency in knowledgebases.
      Behavioral performance was analyzed across 32 effects in an overall omnibus analysis spanning different WM tasks. The data assignments used across all effects were paired groups (difference, p), paired groups (N, t-value), and independent groups (means, SDs). The number of effects is defined as k; Cohen’s d is defined as d.
      To assess factors that might influence response to tACS, we explored moderator variables that might affect behavioral outcomes in subgroup moderator analyses. Performance was further assessed on behavioral tasks with multiple effects. The categorical variables examined were WM task domains (three levels: identifying letters (verbal) vs spatial location vs object recognition (nonverbal), cognitive load on N-Back task (1-Back vs 2-Back vs 3-Back vs 2-Back over 1-Back), accuracy vs reaction time, study design (between-subject vs within-subject), stimulation frequency (Hz), number of sessions (one vs two vs four), stimulated hemisphere (bilateral vs left vs right), stimulation region (frontal vs parietal vs frontoparietal vs frontotemporal), and online vs offline task performance. Meta-regression was performed to explore characteristics of continuous variables, including stimulation duration and participant demographics (mean age, education, percentage female vs male).
      Publication bias was evaluated by visual assessment of the funnel plot, which provides a graphic scatter plot of the effect-size estimates from each study plotted against the result. A relatively symmetrical funnel plot indicates absence of publication bias, whereas an asymmetrical shape indicates bias among the included studies.
      • Mavridis D.
      • Salanti G.
      How to assess publication bias: funnel plot, trim-and-fill method and selection models.
      Egger’s regression was used to quantify a statistical measure of the funnel plot.
      • Egger M.
      • Davey Smith G.
      • Schneider M.
      • Minder C.
      Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test.
      An adjusted rank-correlation test was calculated according to the methods of Begg and Mazumdar.
      • Jones K.T.
      • Arciniega H.
      • Berryhill M.E.
      Replacing tDCS with theta tACS provides selective, but not general WM benefits.
      The classic fail-safe N was used as a measure to identify the number of additional negative studies that would be needed to negate the current findings.
      • Orwin R.G.
      A fail-safe N for effect size in meta-analysis.

      Results

      From the initial data base search, we identified ten articles
      • Hoy K.E.
      • Bailey N.
      • Arnold S.
      • et al.
      The effect of γ-tACS on working memory performance in healthy controls.
      ,
      • Reinhart R.M.G.
      • Nguyen J.A.
      Working memory revived in older adults by synchronizing rhythmic brain circuits.
      ,
      • Jaušovec N.
      • Jaušovec K.
      Increasing working memory capacity with theta transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS).
      ,
      • Meiron O.
      • Lavidor M.
      Prefrontal oscillatory stimulation modulates access to cognitive control references in retrospective metacognitive commentary.
      • Jaušovec N.
      • Jaušovec K.
      • Pahor A.
      The influence of theta transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) on working memory storage and processing functions.
      • Borghini G.
      • Candini M.
      • Filannino C.
      • et al.
      Alpha oscillations are causally linked to inhibitory abilities in ageing.
      • Jones K.T.
      • Arciniega H.
      • Berryhill M.E.
      Replacing tDCS with theta tACS provides selective, but not general WM benefits.
      • Bender M.
      • Romei V.
      • Sauseng P.
      Slow theta tACS of the right parietal cortex enhances contralateral visual working memory capacity.
      • Biel A.L.
      • Sterner E.
      • Röll L.
      • Sauseng P.
      Modulating verbal working memory with fronto-parietal transcranial electric stimulation at theta frequency: does it work?.
      • Thompson L.
      • Khuc J.
      • Saccani M.S.
      • Zokaei N.
      • Cappelletti M.
      Gamma oscillations modulate working memory recall precision.
      that met our inclusion criteria and provided 32 effects (k = 32) included in the meta-analysis. All articles involved tACS, with 16 effects involving subject-specific frequency and 16 effects set at a standard frequency. The overall sample size across all effects included n = 695 healthy participants who underwent tACS during WM task performance (online) or tACS in between task assessments (offline). Study details are shown in Table 1.

      Effects Across all tACS Studies and WM Tasks

      Omnibus Analysis
      The omnibus analysis of overall effects from active tACS across all WM tasks resulted in a significant and moderate improvement in behavioral performance over sham (k = 32; d = 0.514; 95% CI = 0.349–0.680; z = 6.105; p = 0.0001). Analysis of homogeneity indicated that specific study effect sizes were significantly heterogeneous (Q-stat = 91.47; df = 32; p = 0.0001). Given the variability across tasks, study-specific effect sizes, and differences in tACS parameters, moderator analyses were performed to better account for the observed heterogeneity. The study statistics and corresponding forest plot for the omnibus analysis are provided in Figure 2.
      Figure thumbnail gr2
      Figure 2Overall meta-analysis effect size (Cohen’s d omnibus effect = 0.514) of all included tACS studies. Corresponding forest plot shows the effects of favoring active stimulation (> 0) vs favoring sham stimulation (< 0).

      Moderator Analyses

      Effect of Cognitive Load: N-Back Task

      Assessment of cognitive load and its relationship with tACS revealed a significant difference such that 2-Back over 1-Back showed the greatest effect (k = 1, d = 1.709, 95% CI = 0.822–2.597, p <0.0001), followed by 2-Back (k = 4, d = 1.067, 95% CI = 0.376–1.76, p = 0.002), 1-Back (k = 2, d = 0.839, 95% CI = 0.373–1.304, p <0.0001), and 3-Back (k = 2 d = 0.072, 95% CI = -0.53 to 0.67, p = 0.813) (Q-stat = 10.32; df = 3; p = 0.02). This suggests that tACS effects on WM behavior are beneficial for the more challenging 2-Back over 1-Back condition but do not reliably influence the highest-level difficulty (ie, 3-Back [p > 0.05]).

      Task Domains: Verbal, Spatial, and Object

      Analysis of WM task domains included three levels: verbal, spatial, and object. We defined verbal tasks as those that used language-related stimuli, including single letters. Spatial tasks tested subjects on the location of visual stimuli, whereas object tasks tested recognition of sequentially presented visual stimuli. Active tACS had a significant and larger improvement in verbal WM tasks (k = 16; d = 0.720; 95% CI = 0.498–0.942) than in tasks testing spatial location (k = 6; d = 0.321; 95% CI = −0.154 to 0.796) and object recognition (k = 5; d = 0.238; 95% CI = −0.095 to 0.571) (Q-stat = 6.520; df = 2; p = 0.04).

      Task Accuracy vs Reaction Time

      Contrasts assessing accuracy (k = 25; d = 0.622; 95% CI = 0.453–0.790) vs reaction time (k = 7; d = -0.008; 95% CI = -0.247 to 0.231) revealed that accuracy was significantly and moderately enhanced from active tACS, whereas reaction time (d = 0.622) slowed as a function of stimulation (p = 0.0001).

      Number of tACS Sessions

      Contrasts assessing number of sessions indicated a significant, heterogeneous result suggesting that a higher number of sessions imparts greater benefit to WM behavior for active over sham stimulation (four sessions: k = 2; d = 0.750; 95% CI = 0.435–1.063; two sessions: k = 14; d = 0.735; 95% CI = 0.454–1.016; one session: k = 16; d = 0.301; 95% CI = 0.109–0.494) (Q-stat = 9.211; df = 2; p = 0.01).

      Target Region of Stimulation

      Contrasts assessing stimulation region revealed a significant effect between parietal, frontal, frontoparietal, and frontotemporal stimulation (k = 15, 5, 8, 4, respectively), in which parietal received the most benefit (d = 0.742), followed by frontal (d = 0.479), frontoparietal (d = 0.255), and frontotemporal stimulation (d = 0.202) (Q-stat = 9.082; df = 3; p = 0.03).

      Nonsignificant Moderator Variables

      Nonsignificant moderator variables included 1) study design type (within-subject, k = 24; between-subject, k = 8) (Q-stat = 0.001, df = 1; p = 0.973); 2) online (k = 17) vs offline (k = 15) performance (Q-stat = 0.782; df = 1; p = 0.38); 3) waveform phase–in-phase (k = 28) vs antiphase (k = 2) (Q-stat = 1.559; df = 1; p = 0.212); 4) frequency (Hz) range 4 to 40 Hz (Q-stat = 2.848; d = 6; p = 0.83); 5) personalized (Hz) (k = 16) vs standard frequency (k = 16) (Q-stat = 0.470; df = 1; p = 0.50); 6) electrode type (conventional vs HD-tACS) (Q-stat = 1.104; df = 1; p = 0.30); and 7) stimulation hemisphere (bilateral vs left vs right; k = 5, 17, 10) (Q-stat = 0.133; df = 2; p = 0.071).

      Meta-Regression for Continuous Variables

      No significant differences were observed for stimulation duration (12-, 15-, 20-, 25-minutes) (z = −1.33; p = 0.19). Participant demographics did not reveal significant moderation of effect size by age (z = −1.10; p = 0.27) (mean age = 30.56 years; range = 20.5–69.6) or education (z = −1.22; p = 0.22; mean education = 16.7 years). In addition, young (k = 26) vs older adults (k = 6) was not a significant moderating variable (p = 0.364). Meta-regression revealed a significant moderation of effect size by percentage of female vs male participants (z = 1.95; p = 0.05), suggesting that studies with a higher number of female participants may benefit more from active tACS than from sham.

      Publication Bias

      Evaluation of publication bias revealed significant Begg (one-tailed p = 0.0003) and Egger (one-tailed p = 0.00018) tests, indicating the possibility of bias within this sample of literature.
      • Begg C.B.
      • Mazumdar M.
      Operating characteristics of a rank correlation test for publication bias.
      Trim-and-fill analyses identified five putative outlier effects. If excluded, they only minimally reduced the omnibus effect size (d = 0.34).
      • Cohen J.
      Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences.
      ,,
      • Schäfer T.
      • Schwarz M.A.
      The meaningfulness of effect sizes in psychological research: differences between sub-disciplines and the impact of potential biases.
      Finally, the calculation of the classic fail-safe N indicated that 573 negative or “null” results would be needed to negate the present findings. Figure 3 displays the funnel plot for all included studies.
      Figure thumbnail gr3
      Figure 3Funnel plot displays tACS effects for the assessment of publication bias.

      Discussion

      This systematic meta-analysis explored the efficacy of tACS in the enhancement of WM performance in healthy adults. Results revealed a significant, heterogeneous positive effect of active tACS in improving WM performance over sham. A previous meta-analysis assessed tACS on visual cognition
      • Bullard B.
      • Levina V.
      • Grover S.
      • Reinhart R.
      Effects of transcranial alternating current stimulation on visual cognition: a systematic review and meta-analysis.
      but assessed different cognitive domains with no pooled effect size.
      • Klink K.
      • Paßmann S.
      • Kasten F.H.
      • Peter J.
      The modulation of cognitive performance with transcranial alternating current stimulation: a systematic review of frequency-specific effects.
      This meta-analysis extends the literature by assessing the effectiveness of tACS on WM behavioral performance and factors that might modulate stimulation effects, which, to our knowledge, has not been the primary focus of previous meta-analyses. In subanalyses, we explored potential moderator variables that could affect tACS response, including task-dependent effects, variations in stimulation parameters, and participant demographics. Collectively, these data suggest that active tACS may enhance WM performance in healthy individuals over sham.
      As we predicted, a task-dependent effect of tACS was identified on the N-Back task, suggesting cognitive load may be important for stimulation response. The load effect was specific to 2-Back over 1-Back condition (which targets attention but lacks the manipulation aspects of WM), whereas the 3-Back condition was not significant. This indicates that capacity of tACS-induced enhancement may depend on the nature of the task, with limits that might relate to WM network ceiling effects, given the nonsignificant 3-Back condition. Previous research has shown that task-dependent effects of tES cognitive enhancements relate to the nature and cognitive load of the task being performed during stimulation;
      • Nissim N.R.
      • O’Shea A.
      • Indahlastari A.
      • et al.
      Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation paired with cognitive training on functional connectivity of the working memory network in older adults.
      ,
      • Hoy K.E.
      • Bailey N.
      • Arnold S.
      • et al.
      The effect of γ-tACS on working memory performance in healthy controls.
      ,
      • Gill J.
      • Shah-Basak P.P.
      • Hamilton R.
      It’s the thought that counts: examining the task-dependent effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on executive function.
      ,
      • Santarnecchi E.
      • Polizzotto N.R.
      • Godone M.
      • et al.
      Frequency-dependent enhancement of fluid intelligence induced by transcranial oscillatory potentials.
      this finding is also corroborated with state-dependent effects of tACS (ie, physiological state and fluctuations in neural activity) that have been suggested to occur in the motor system.
      • Feurra M.
      • Pasqualetti P.
      • Bianco G.
      • Santarnecchi E.
      • Rossi A.
      • Rossi S.
      State-dependent effects of transcranial oscillatory currents on the motor system: what you think matters.
      These results also align with functional neuroimaging evidence indicating that neural activation of WM-related brain regions correlates with the cognitive demands of a task.
      • Nagel I.E.
      • Preuschhof C.
      • Li S.C.
      • et al.
      Load modulation of BOLD response and connectivity predicts working memory performance in younger and older adults.
      ,
      • Heinzel S.
      • Lorenz R.C.
      • Brockhaus W.R.
      • et al.
      Working memory load-dependent brain response predicts behavioral training gains in older adults.
      Verbal and nonverbal WM tasks are supported by different neural processes,
      • Postle B.R.
      • Desposito M.
      • Corkin S.
      Effects of verbal and nonverbal interference on spatial and object visual working memory.
      which could be differentially affected by tACS. Thus, we examined differences in task domain as potential moderating factors across three levels (verbal, spatial, and object stimuli). We identified significantly larger improvements in verbal than in spatial and object recognition tasks. Previous neuroimaging studies point to hemispheric lateralization between verbal vs spatial WM in left hemisphere (LH) vs right hemisphere (RH), respectively.
      • Nagel B.J.
      • Herting M.M.
      • Maxwell E.C.
      • Bruno R.
      • Fair D.
      Hemispheric lateralization of verbal and spatial working memory during adolescence.
      Because site specific effects may alter the impact of tACS for different WM subdomains, our results should be interpreted cautiously; very few studies in our analysis compared performance on the same behavioral task paired with stimulation at different sites.
      Given the bihemispheric network of brain regions that are known to subserve WM, we examined hemisphere and stimulation region as moderator variables. Stimulation region was a significant moderator for active tACS—parietal lobe had the strongest effect on WM behavior, followed by frontal, frontoparietal, and frontotemporal. Hemisphere of stimulation (LH, RH, bilateral) did not significantly moderate tACS effects. The parietal region is understood to be an essential node in the WM network,
      • Koenigs M.
      • Barbey A.K.
      • Postle B.R.
      • Grafman J.
      Superior parietal cortex is critical for the manipulation of information in working memory.
      with involvement in short-term storage and retrieval of phonologically coded verbal information.
      • Jonides J.
      • Schumacher E.H.
      • Smith E.E.
      • et al.
      The role of parietal cortex in verbal working memory.
      Patients with superior parietal lesions exhibit deficits when WM tasks require manipulation of information and show normal performance on rehearsal/retrieval processes, which indicates the critical nature of the parietal lobe during manipulation of WM information.
      • Koenigs M.
      • Barbey A.K.
      • Postle B.R.
      • Grafman J.
      Superior parietal cortex is critical for the manipulation of information in working memory.
      Our results suggest that parietal tACS is associated with improvement in several WM functions. This indicates that stimulation to other brain regions may be less effective. These findings underscore the importance of determining appropriate and optimal targets for WM task enhancement through tACS.
      Across all effects, accuracy and reaction time were influenced by active stimulation over sham; accuracy significantly improved, whereas reaction time, a proxy for processing speed, slowed in response to stimulation. This is broadly consistent with previous research exploring tACS for cognitive remediation in healthy older adults, in which accuracy, but not reaction time, has been shown to be enhanced by active stimulation.
      • Reinhart R.M.G.
      • Nguyen J.A.
      Working memory revived in older adults by synchronizing rhythmic brain circuits.
      This is also consistent with previous studies using tDCS.
      • Nissim N.R.
      • O’Shea A.
      • Indahlastari A.
      • et al.
      Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation paired with cognitive training on functional connectivity of the working memory network in older adults.
      ,
      • Seo M.H.
      • Park S.H.
      • Seo J.H.
      • Kim Y.H.
      • Ko M.H.
      Improvement of the working memory by transcranial direct current stimulation in healthy older adults.
      However, the finding could represent a speed-accuracy tradeoff whereby, owing to the WM benefits induced by stimulation, individuals are able to respond with fewer errors but at the cost of responding more slowly.
      • Zimmerman M.
      Speed-accuracy tradeoff.
      Stimulation parameters including number of sessions and frequency (Hz) (standardized vs personalized [Hz]), are important factors that can affect tACS response. Consistently with previous studies,
      • Fregni F.
      • Boggio P.S.
      • Nitsche M.A.
      • Rigonatti S.P.
      • Pascual-Leone A.
      Cognitive effects of repeated sessions of transcranial direct current stimulation in patients with depression [1].
      • Monte-Silva K.
      • Kuo M.F.
      • Hessenthaler S.
      • et al.
      Induction of late LTP-like plasticity in the human motor cortex by repeated non-invasive brain stimulation.
      • Boggio P.S.
      • Nunes A.
      • Rigonatti S.P.
      • Nitsche M.A.
      • Pascual-Leone A.
      • Fregni F.
      Repeated sessions of noninvasive brain DC stimulation is associated with motor function improvement in stroke patients.
      we found that number of sessions (four vs two vs one) significantly moderated response to stimulation; a higher number of sessions was associated with more robust effects. This finding may relate to underlying mechanisms of neuroplasticity; studies of tES have shown that repeated sessions of stimulation may produce stable long-term changes in neuroplasticity through mechanisms like long-term potentiation.
      • Monte-Silva K.
      • Kuo M.F.
      • Hessenthaler S.
      • et al.
      Induction of late LTP-like plasticity in the human motor cortex by repeated non-invasive brain stimulation.
      • Boggio P.S.
      • Nunes A.
      • Rigonatti S.P.
      • Nitsche M.A.
      • Pascual-Leone A.
      • Fregni F.
      Repeated sessions of noninvasive brain DC stimulation is associated with motor function improvement in stroke patients.
      • Monte-Silva K.
      • Kuo M.F.
      • Liebetanz D.
      • Paulus W.
      • Nitsche M.A.
      Shaping the optimal repetition interval for cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS).
      Stimulation frequency, including standardized vs personalized Hz, did not significantly moderate tACS effects. It has been suggested that personalized frequency may confer greater benefits in behavior than does a standard frequency across participants.
      • Reinhart R.M.G.
      • Nguyen J.A.
      Working memory revived in older adults by synchronizing rhythmic brain circuits.
      However, our results suggest no significant difference between EEG-informed tACS vs standardized frequency across healthy participants. This may not necessarily mean that personalized tACS is less effective; across studies, different approaches are used to determine endogenous peak frequency (eg, EEG-triggered transcranial magnetic stimulation, closed-loop NIBS during task/rest).
      • Klink K.
      • Paßmann S.
      • Kasten F.H.
      • Peter J.
      The modulation of cognitive performance with transcranial alternating current stimulation: a systematic review of frequency-specific effects.
      ,
      • Pahor A.
      • Jaušovec N.
      The effects of theta and gamma tacs on working memory and electrophysiology.
      ,
      • Reinhart R.M.G.
      • Nguyen J.A.
      Working memory revived in older adults by synchronizing rhythmic brain circuits.
      ,
      • Aktürk T.
      • de Graaf T.A.
      • Güntekin B.
      • Hanoğlu L.
      • Sack A.T.
      Enhancing memory capacity by experimentally slowing theta frequency oscillations using combined EEG-tACS.
      • Bjekić J.
      • Paunovic D.
      • Živanović M.
      • Stanković M.
      • Griskova-Bulanova I.
      • Filipović S.R.
      Determining the individual theta frequency for associative memory targeted personalized transcranial brain stimulation.
      • Aktürk T.
      • de Graaf T.A.
      • Erdal F.
      • Sack A.T.
      • Güntekin B.
      Oscillatory delta and theta frequencies differentially support multiple items encoding to optimize memory performance during the digit span task.
      • Živanović M.
      • Bjekić J.
      • Konstantinović U.
      • Filipović S.R.
      Effects of online parietal transcranial electric stimulation on associative memory: a direct comparison between tDCS, theta tACS, and theta-oscillatory tDCS.
      Different methods may impart variability in personalization of tACS. More insight is needed to reduce the potential variability of EEG-informed tACS effects across studies. In addition, participant demographics may be a confounding factor; most studies included relatively young adults with high performance rates and ceiling effects compared with older adults with normal age-related decline.
      Other parameters important to tACS response include duration of stimulation, online or offline task performance, in-phase vs antiphase waveforms, and conventional vs HD-tACS. Duration of stimulation (12-,15-, 20-, 25-minutes) was not associated with significant differences in tACS effects. This may indicate that the maximum benefit from tACS can be achieved in a short stimulation session in healthy young adults. Variables such as online (during) or offline (after stimulation) performance, in-phase vs antiphase, and conventional vs HD-tACS were also not significant moderators of response to tACS.
      Demographic features such as age, sex, and education have the potential to influence response to stimulation and were examined as covariates using meta-regression. Mean age and education were nonsignificant factors with respect to stimulation effects. The age range across all effects was 20.5 to 69.6 years. We categorized subjects as young vs older adults (26 vs 6 effects, respectively) to examine potential age-related differences in tACS response but observed none. Sex was a significant covariate in response to stimulation; a higher percentage of female participants in studies was associated with greater WM performance. However, this finding could be driven largely by the higher number of women in this particular sample, and not an actual biological difference in response to tACS.
      This study had several limitations. Although our criteria were broad, the final number of studies that met inclusion was low, and stimulation protocols varied. We acknowledge that methodologic heterogeneity across tACS protocols limits the ability to identify the most beneficial strategy. To be comprehensive, we performed several moderator analyses in which only a small number of effects could be compared. One area in which we think more data are needed is the determination of whether stimulation at personalized tACS frequencies vs standardized frequency influences stimulation effects in samples of young healthy adults. Specific regions of the WM network that are preferentially involved in particular aspects of WM may be differentially influenced by tACS (ie, variability of functional connectivity between regions within the WM network could affect response to stimulation). Future research combining tACS with neuroimaging techniques (eg, EEG, functional magnetic resonance imaging [MRI]; structural MRI) may provide greater insights into brain processing during WM performance and aid in the optimization of targeted stimulation sites for tACS WM enhancement.

      Conclusions

      In summary, we identified a significant, heterogeneous effect of tACS on the enhancement of WM performance and several factors that may affect response to stimulation. Future research in this area will need to address substantive gaps in the existing data by conducting studies with larger subject samples, increasing the focus on important parameter settings and protocol optimization, and further examining structure-function relationships mediating the effects of stimulation on specific WM abilities. Future studies that explore cross-frequency coupling during tACS and WM performance may provide greater guidance toward protocol optimization. Nonetheless, this meta-analysis provides support for the use of tACS as a tool to interrogate and improve WM and foundational evidence to support the exploration of tACS as a potential intervention for clinical populations with WM deficits.

      Authorship Statements

      Nicole R. Nissim was responsible for the project, literature search, data analyses, figures, and tables and prepared the original draft of the manuscript. Darrian C. McAfee, Shanna Edwards, Amara Prato, and Jennifer X. Lin aided in screening articles and compiling data. Jennifer X. Lin and Zhiye Lu aided in the manuscript draft and tables. H. Branch Coslett and Roy H. Hamilton were responsible for intellectual contribution to screening and extracted data, contributed to analyses, and aided in manuscript edits. All authors reviewed and approved the final manuscript.

      References

        • Baddeley A.
        Working memory.
        Curr Biol. 2010; 20: R136-R140https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2009.12.014
        • Jaeggi S.M.
        • Buschkuehl M.
        • Jonides J.
        • Perrig W.J.
        Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory.
        Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008; 105: 6829-6833https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0801268105
        • Au J.
        • Sheehan E.
        • Tsai N.
        • Duncan G.J.
        • Buschkuehl M.
        • Jaeggi S.M.
        Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory: a meta-analysis.
        Psychon Bull Rev. 2015; 22: 366-377https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0699-x
        • Schupf N.
        • Tang M.X.
        • Albert S.M.
        • et al.
        Decline in cognitive and functional skills increases mortality risk in nondemented elderly.
        Neurology. 2005; 65: 1218-1226https://doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000180970.07386.cb
        • Mograbi D.C.
        • Faria Cde A.
        • Fichman H.C.
        • Paradela E.M.P.
        • Lourenço R.A.
        Relationship between activities of daily living and cognitive ability in a sample of older adults with heterogeneous educational level.
        Ann Indian Acad Neurol. 2014; 17: 71-76https://doi.org/10.4103/0972-2327.128558
        • Nissim N.R.
        • O’Shea A.
        • Indahlastari A.
        • et al.
        Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation paired with cognitive training on functional connectivity of the working memory network in older adults.
        Front Aging Neurosci. 2019; 11: 340https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2019.00340
        • Nissim N.R.
        • O’Shea A.
        • Indahlastari A.
        • et al.
        Effects of in-scanner bilateral frontal tDCS on functional connectivity of the working memory network in older adults.
        Front Aging Neurosci. 2019; 11: 51https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2019.00051
        • Ditye T.
        • Jacobson L.
        • Walsh V.
        • Lavidor M.
        Modulating behavioral inhibition by tDCS combined with cognitive training.
        Exp Brain Res. 2012; 219: 363-368https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-012-3098-4
        • Park S.H.
        • Seo J.H.
        • Kim Y.H.
        • Ko M.H.
        Long-term effects of transcranial direct current stimulation combined with computer-assisted cognitive training in healthy older adults.
        NeuroReport. 2014; 25: 122-126https://doi.org/10.1097/WNR.0000000000000080
        • Meinzer M.
        • Lindenberg R.
        • Antonenko D.
        • Flaisch T.
        • Flöel A.
        Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation temporarily reverses age-associated cognitive decline and functional brain activity changes.
        J Neurosci. 2013; 33: 12470-12478https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5743-12.2013
        • Dhaynaut M.
        • Cappon D.
        • Paciorek R.
        • et al.
        Effects of modulating gamma oscillations via 40Hz transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) on Tau PET imaging in mild to moderate Alzheimer’s Disease.
        J Nucl Med. 2020; 61: 340
        • Benussi A.
        • Cantoni V.
        • Cotelli M.S.
        • et al.
        Exposure to gamma tACS in Alzheimer’s disease: a randomized, double-blind, sham-controlled, crossover, pilot study.
        Brain Stimul. 2021; 14: 531-540https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2021.03.007
        • Senkowski D.
        • Sobirey R.
        • Haslacher D.
        • Soekadar S.R.
        Boosting working memory: uncovering the differential effects of tDCS and tACS.
        Cereb Cortex Commun. 2022; 3tgac018https://doi.org/10.1093/texcom/tgac018
        • Antonenko D.
        • Faxel M.
        • Grittner U.
        • Lavidor M.
        • Floël A.
        Effects of transcranial alternating current stimulation on cognitive functions in healthy young and older adults.
        Neural Plast. 2016; 20164274127https://doi.org/10.1155/2016/4274127
        • Elyamany O.
        • Leicht G.
        • Herrmann C.S.
        • Mulert C.
        Transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS): from basic mechanisms towards first applications in psychiatry.
        Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2021; 271: 135-156https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-020-01209-9
        • Antal A.
        • Paulus W.
        Transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS).
        Front Hum Neurosci. 2013; 7: 317https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00317
        • Alekseichuk I.
        • Turi Z.
        • Veit S.
        • Paulus W.
        Model-driven neuromodulation of the right posterior region promotes encoding of long-term memories.
        Brain Stimul. 2020; 13: 474-483https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2019.12.019
        • Herrmann C.S.
        Modeling-informed tACS allows shaping oscillatory activity in specific brain networks.
        Brain Stimul. 2017; 10: 387https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2017.01.144
        • Vosskuhl J.
        • Strüber D.
        • Herrmann C.S.
        Transcranial alternating current stimulation. entrainment and function control of neuronal networks.
        Nervenarzt. 2015; 86 (Article in German): 1516-1522https://doi.org/10.1007/s00115-015-4317-6
        • Vosskuhl J.
        • Strüber D.
        • Herrmann C.S.
        Non-invasive brain stimulation: A paradigm shift in understanding brain oscillations.
        Front Hum Neurosci. 2018; 12: 211https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00211
        • Fröhlich F.
        • McCormick D.A.
        Endogenous electric fields may guide neocortical network activity.
        Neuron. 2010; 67: 129-143https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.06.005
        • Klink K.
        • Paßmann S.
        • Kasten F.H.
        • Peter J.
        The modulation of cognitive performance with transcranial alternating current stimulation: a systematic review of frequency-specific effects.
        Brain Sci. 2020; 10: 1-33https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10120932
        • Ruhnau P.
        • Neuling T.
        • Fuscá M.
        • Herrmann C.S.
        • Demarchi G.
        • Weisz N.
        Eyes wide shut: transcranial alternating current stimulation drives alpha rhythm in a state dependent manner.
        Sci Rep. 2016; 627138https://doi.org/10.1038/srep27138
        • Neuling T.
        • Ruhnau P.
        • Weisz N.
        • Herrmann C.S.
        • Demarchi G.
        Faith and oscillations recovered: on analyzing EEG/MEG signals during tACS.
        Neuroimage. 2017; 147: 960-963https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.022
        • Feurra M.
        • Pasqualetti P.
        • Bianco G.
        • Santarnecchi E.
        • Rossi A.
        • Rossi S.
        State-dependent effects of transcranial oscillatory currents on the motor system: what you think matters.
        J Neurosci. 2013; 33: 17483-17489https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1414-13.2013
        • Nakazono H.
        • Ogata K.
        • Kuroda T.
        • Tobimatsu S.
        Phase and frequency-dependent effects of transcranial alternating current stimulation on motor cortical excitability.
        PLoS One. 2016; 11e0162521https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0162521
        • Pahor A.
        • Jaušovec N.
        The effects of theta and gamma tacs on working memory and electrophysiology.
        Front Hum Neurosci. 2018; 11: 651https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00651
        • Antal A.
        • Paulus W.
        • Nitsche M.A.
        Principle and mechanisms of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS).
        J Pain Manag. 2009; 2: 249-258
        • Tavakoli A.V.
        • Yun K.
        Transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) mechanisms and protocols.
        Front Cell Neurosci. 2017; 11: 214https://doi.org/10.3389/fncel.2017.00214
        • Roux F.
        • Uhlhaas P.J.
        Working memory and neural oscillations: α-γ versus θ-γ codes for distinct WM information?.
        Trends Cogn Sci. 2014; 18: 16-25https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.10.010
        • Hoy K.E.
        • Bailey N.
        • Arnold S.
        • et al.
        The effect of γ-tACS on working memory performance in healthy controls.
        Brain Cogn. 2015; 101: 51-56https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2015.11.002
        • Reinhart R.M.G.
        • Nguyen J.A.
        Working memory revived in older adults by synchronizing rhythmic brain circuits.
        Nat Neurosci. 2019; 22: 820-827https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-019-0371-x
        • Lisman J.E.
        • Jensen O.
        The θ-γ neural code.
        Neuron. 2013; 77: 1002-1016https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2013.03.007
        • Sauseng P.
        • Klimesch W.
        • Heise K.F.
        • et al.
        Brain oscillatory substrates of visual short-term memory capacity.
        Curr Biol. 2009; 19: 1846-1852https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2009.08.062
        • Axmacher N.
        • Henseler M.M.
        • Jensen O.
        • Weinreich I.
        • Elger C.E.
        • Fell J.
        Cross-frequency coupling supports multi-item working memory in the human hippocampus.
        Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010; 107: 3228-3233https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0911531107
        • Fell J.
        • Axmacher N.
        The role of phase synchronization in memory processes.
        Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011; 12: 105-118https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2979
        • Sauseng P.
        • Griesmayr B.
        • Freunberger R.
        • Klimesch W.
        Control mechanisms in working memory: a possible function of EEG theta oscillations.
        Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2010; 34: 1015-1022https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.12.006
        • Alekseichuk I.
        • Turi Z.
        • Amador de Lara G.
        • Antal A.
        • Paulus W.
        Spatial working memory in humans depends on theta and high gamma synchronization in the prefrontal cortex.
        Curr Biol. 2016; 26: 1513-1521https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.04.035
        • Alekseichuk I.
        • Pabel S.C.
        • Antal A.
        • Paulus W.
        Intrahemispheric theta rhythm desynchronization impairs working memory.
        Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2017; 35: 147-158https://doi.org/10.3233/RNN-160714
        • Jaušovec N.
        • Jaušovec K.
        Increasing working memory capacity with theta transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS).
        Biol Psychol. 2014; 96: 42-47https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.11.006
        • Bullard B.
        • Levina V.
        • Grover S.
        • Reinhart R.
        Effects of transcranial alternating current stimulation on visual cognition: a systematic review and meta-analysis.
        Journal of Vision. 2020; 20: 541
        • Vosskuhl J.
        • Huster R.J.
        • Herrmann C.S.
        Increase in short-term memory capacity induced by down-regulating individual theta frequency via transcranial alternating current stimulation.
        Front Hum Neurosci. 2015; 9: 257https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00257
        • Honkanen R.
        • Rouhinen S.
        • Wang S.H.
        • Palva J.M.
        • Palva S.
        Gamma oscillations underlie the maintenance of feature-specific information and the contents of visual working memory.
        Cereb Cortex. 2015; 25: 3788-3801https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhu263
        • Howard M.W.
        • Rizzuto D.S.
        • Caplan J.B.
        • et al.
        Gamma oscillations correlate with working memory load in humans.
        Cereb Cortex. 2003; 13: 1369-1374https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhg084
        • Guerra A.
        • Asci F.
        • Zampogna A.
        • et al.
        Gamma-transcranial alternating current stimulation and theta-burst stimulation: inter-subject variability and the role of BDNF.
        Clin Neurophysiol. 2020; 131: 2691-2699https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2020.08.017
        • Moher D.
        • Liberati A.
        • Tetzlaff J.
        • Altman D.G.
        • PRISMA Group
        Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses: the PRISMA statement.
        PLoS Med. 2009; 6e1000097https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097
        • Page M.J.
        • McKenzie J.E.
        • Bossuyt P.M.
        • et al.
        The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews.
        Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 2021; 134: 178-189https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2021.03.001
        • Meiron O.
        • Lavidor M.
        Prefrontal oscillatory stimulation modulates access to cognitive control references in retrospective metacognitive commentary.
        Clin Neurophysiol. 2014; 125: 77-82https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2013.06.013
        • Jaušovec N.
        • Jaušovec K.
        • Pahor A.
        The influence of theta transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) on working memory storage and processing functions.
        Acta Psychol (Amst). 2014; 146: 1-6https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.11.011
        • Borghini G.
        • Candini M.
        • Filannino C.
        • et al.
        Alpha oscillations are causally linked to inhibitory abilities in ageing.
        J Neurosci. 2018; 38: 4418-4429https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1285-17.2018
        • Jones K.T.
        • Arciniega H.
        • Berryhill M.E.
        Replacing tDCS with theta tACS provides selective, but not general WM benefits.
        Brain Res. 2019; 1720146324https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2019.146324
        • Bender M.
        • Romei V.
        • Sauseng P.
        Slow theta tACS of the right parietal cortex enhances contralateral visual working memory capacity.
        Brain Topogr. 2019; 32: 477-481https://doi.org/10.1007/s10548-019-00702-2
        • Biel A.L.
        • Sterner E.
        • Röll L.
        • Sauseng P.
        Modulating verbal working memory with fronto-parietal transcranial electric stimulation at theta frequency: does it work?.
        Eur J Neurosci. 2022; 55: 405-425https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.15563
        • Thompson L.
        • Khuc J.
        • Saccani M.S.
        • Zokaei N.
        • Cappelletti M.
        Gamma oscillations modulate working memory recall precision.
        Exp Brain Res. 2021; 239: 2711-2724https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-021-06051-6
        • Riley R.D.
        • Higgins J.P.T.
        • Deeks J.J.
        Interpretation of random effects meta-analyses.
        BMJ. 2011; 342: d549https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.d549
        • Cohen J.
        Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences.
        Academic Press, 1988
        • Hedges L.V.
        • Olkin I.
        Statistical Methods for Meta-Analysis.
        Academic Press, 1985https://doi.org/10.2307/1164953
        • Grant J.
        • Hunter A.
        Measuring inconsistency in knowledgebases.
        J Intell Inf Syst. 2006; 27: 159-184https://doi.org/10.1007/s10844-006-2974-4
        • Mavridis D.
        • Salanti G.
        How to assess publication bias: funnel plot, trim-and-fill method and selection models.
        Evid Based Ment Health. 2014; 17: 30https://doi.org/10.1136/eb-2013-101699
        • Egger M.
        • Davey Smith G.
        • Schneider M.
        • Minder C.
        Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test.
        BMJ. 1997; 315: 629-634
        • Orwin R.G.
        A fail-safe N for effect size in meta-analysis.
        J Educ Stat. 1983; 8: 157-159
        • Begg C.B.
        • Mazumdar M.
        Operating characteristics of a rank correlation test for publication bias.
        Biometrics. 1994; 50: 1088-1101
        • Becker L.A.
        Effect Size (ES).
        (. 2000. Accessed February 3, 2023. Retrieved from)
        • Schäfer T.
        • Schwarz M.A.
        The meaningfulness of effect sizes in psychological research: differences between sub-disciplines and the impact of potential biases.
        Front Psychol. 2019; 10: 813https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00813
        • Gill J.
        • Shah-Basak P.P.
        • Hamilton R.
        It’s the thought that counts: examining the task-dependent effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on executive function.
        Brain Stimul. 2015; 8: 253-259https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2014.10.018
        • Santarnecchi E.
        • Polizzotto N.R.
        • Godone M.
        • et al.
        Frequency-dependent enhancement of fluid intelligence induced by transcranial oscillatory potentials.
        Curr Biol. 2013; 23: 1449-1453https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.06.022
        • Nagel I.E.
        • Preuschhof C.
        • Li S.C.
        • et al.
        Load modulation of BOLD response and connectivity predicts working memory performance in younger and older adults.
        J Cogn Neurosci. 2011; 23: 2030-2045https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2010.21560
        • Heinzel S.
        • Lorenz R.C.
        • Brockhaus W.R.
        • et al.
        Working memory load-dependent brain response predicts behavioral training gains in older adults.
        J Neurosci. 2014; 34: 1224-1233https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2463-13.2014
        • Postle B.R.
        • Desposito M.
        • Corkin S.
        Effects of verbal and nonverbal interference on spatial and object visual working memory.
        Mem Cognit. 2005; 33: 203-212
        • Nagel B.J.
        • Herting M.M.
        • Maxwell E.C.
        • Bruno R.
        • Fair D.
        Hemispheric lateralization of verbal and spatial working memory during adolescence.
        Brain Cogn. 2013; 82: 58-68https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2013.02.007
        • Koenigs M.
        • Barbey A.K.
        • Postle B.R.
        • Grafman J.
        Superior parietal cortex is critical for the manipulation of information in working memory.
        J Neurosci. 2009; 29: 14980-14986https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3706-09.2009
        • Jonides J.
        • Schumacher E.H.
        • Smith E.E.
        • et al.
        The role of parietal cortex in verbal working memory.
        J Neurosci. 1998; 18: 5026-5034
        • Seo M.H.
        • Park S.H.
        • Seo J.H.
        • Kim Y.H.
        • Ko M.H.
        Improvement of the working memory by transcranial direct current stimulation in healthy older adults.
        J Korean Acad Rehab Med. 2011; 35: 201-206
        • Zimmerman M.
        Speed-accuracy tradeoff.
        in: Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology. Springer, 2011: 2344https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-56782-2_1247-3
        • Fregni F.
        • Boggio P.S.
        • Nitsche M.A.
        • Rigonatti S.P.
        • Pascual-Leone A.
        Cognitive effects of repeated sessions of transcranial direct current stimulation in patients with depression [1].
        Depress Anxiety. 2006; 23: 482-484https://doi.org/10.1002/da.20201
        • Monte-Silva K.
        • Kuo M.F.
        • Hessenthaler S.
        • et al.
        Induction of late LTP-like plasticity in the human motor cortex by repeated non-invasive brain stimulation.
        Brain Stimul. 2013; 6: 424-432https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2012.04.011
        • Boggio P.S.
        • Nunes A.
        • Rigonatti S.P.
        • Nitsche M.A.
        • Pascual-Leone A.
        • Fregni F.
        Repeated sessions of noninvasive brain DC stimulation is associated with motor function improvement in stroke patients.
        Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2007; 25: 123-129
        • Monte-Silva K.
        • Kuo M.F.
        • Liebetanz D.
        • Paulus W.
        • Nitsche M.A.
        Shaping the optimal repetition interval for cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS).
        J Neurophysiol. 2010; 103: 1735-1740https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00924.2009
        • Aktürk T.
        • de Graaf T.A.
        • Güntekin B.
        • Hanoğlu L.
        • Sack A.T.
        Enhancing memory capacity by experimentally slowing theta frequency oscillations using combined EEG-tACS.
        Sci Rep. 2022; 1214199https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-18665-z
        • Bjekić J.
        • Paunovic D.
        • Živanović M.
        • Stanković M.
        • Griskova-Bulanova I.
        • Filipović S.R.
        Determining the individual theta frequency for associative memory targeted personalized transcranial brain stimulation.
        J Pers Med. 2022; 12: 1367https://doi.org/10.3390/jpm12091367
        • Aktürk T.
        • de Graaf T.A.
        • Erdal F.
        • Sack A.T.
        • Güntekin B.
        Oscillatory delta and theta frequencies differentially support multiple items encoding to optimize memory performance during the digit span task.
        Neuroimage. 2022; 263119650https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119650
        • Živanović M.
        • Bjekić J.
        • Konstantinović U.
        • Filipović S.R.
        Effects of online parietal transcranial electric stimulation on associative memory: a direct comparison between tDCS, theta tACS, and theta-oscillatory tDCS.
        Sci Rep. 2022; 1214091https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-18376-5